Mirror test

The mirror test is a measure of self-awareness, as animals either possess or lack the ability to recognize themselves in a mirror.

The test was developed by Gordon Gallup Jr. in 1970,[1][2] based in part on observations made by Charles Darwin.[3][4] While visiting a zoo, Darwin held a mirror up to an orangutan and recorded the animal's reaction, which included making a series of facial expressions. Darwin noted that the significance of these expressions was ambiguous, and could either signify that the primate was making expressions at what it perceived to be another animal, or it could be playing a sort of game with a new toy. Gallup reenacted Darwin's initial experiment with two male and two female wild preadolescent chimpanzees, who had presumably never come into contact with mirrors or reflective surfaces. First each chimpanzee was put into a room by itself for two days. Next a full length mirror was placed in the room for a total of 80 hours in variant distances from the cage - starting farther away and moving closer. A multitude of behaviors were recorded upon introducing the mirrors to these wild chimpanzees. At first the chimpanzees made threatening gestures at their own images, they saw their own reflection as a threat. However after some time the chimpanzees used their own projected images for self directed responding - such as grooming parts of the body before unseen without a mirror, picking their noses, making faces, and blowing bubbles at their own reflections. To even further his findings of self recognition in chimpanzees, Gallup postulated another component to the experiment - manipulating the chimpanzee's appearance and observing the reaction.

Gallup built on these observations by devising a test that attempts to gauge self-awareness by determining whether an animal can recognize its own reflection in a mirror as an image of itself. This is accomplished by surreptitiously marking the animal with two odourless dye spots. The test spot is on a part of the animal that would be visible in front of a mirror, while the control spot is in an accessible but hidden part of the animal's body. Scientists observe that the animal reacts in a manner consistent with it being aware that the test dye is located on its own body while ignoring the control dye. Such behaviour includes turning and adjusting of the body in order to better view the marking in the mirror, or poking at the marking on its own body with a limb while viewing the mirror.

At first, even animals that are capable of passing the mirror test respond as the orangutan described by Darwin.[4] Also young children and people who have been blind from birth but have their sight restored, initially react as if their reflection in the mirror was another person.[3][4]

Contents

Animals that have been observed to pass

Animals that have passed the mirror test include:

Initially, it was thought that gorillas did not pass the test, but there are now several well-documented reports of gorillas (such as Koko[12]) passing the test. Humans tend to fail the mirror test until they are about 18 months old, or what psychoanalysts call the "mirror stage".[13][3][4]

Animals that only pass different versions of the test or other tests involving mirrors

Primates other than the great apes have so far failed the mirror test, though rhesus macaques demonstrated a behavior indicative of at least a partial self-awareness.[14] In 1981, Epstein, Lanza and Skinner published a paper in the journal Science in which they argued that the pigeon also passes the mirror test.[15][16] A pigeon was trained to look in a mirror to find a response key behind it which the pigeon then turned to peck - food was the consequence of a correct choice (i.e., the pigeon learned to use a mirror to find critical elements of its environment). Next, the pigeon was trained to peck at dots placed on its feathers; food was, again, the consequence of touching the dot. The latter training was accomplished in the absence of the mirror. The final test was placing a small bib on the pigeon - enough to cover a dot placed on its lower belly. A control period without the mirror present yielded no pecking at the dot. When the mirror was revealed, the pigeon became active, looked in the mirror and then tried to peck on the dot under the bib. It is true that untrained pigeons have never been able to pass the mirror test.[17] However, pigeons do not normally have access to mirrors and do not have the necessary experiences to use them. Giving the pigeons this experience did not guarantee that they would pass the mirror test; the pigeon never pecked dots on its own body in the presence of the mirror (until the final test).

Capuchin monkeys react to their reflection either with hostility or affection, and mark test experiments have shown that they are incapable of spontaneous mirror self-recognition.[18] Similar tests performed using video technology support these findings, but suggest that the monkeys possess the raw input systems required for explicit self-recognition.[19]

Pigs are able to the use information seen in a mirror but do not show evidence of self-recognition. In an experiment, 7 of the 8 pigs tested were able to find a bowl of food hidden behind a wall using a mirror. The eighth pig looked behind the mirror for the food.[20]

Discussion

There is some debate as to the value and interpretation of results of the mirror test.[3] While this test has been extensively conducted on primates, there is debate as to the value of the test as applied to animals who rely primarily on senses other than vision.[3] Adaptations of the mirror test have been made in other modalities, such as scent. For instance, biologist Marc Bekoff developed a paradigm using dog urine for testing self-awareness in canines.[3][4]

Proponents of the hard problem of consciousness claim that the mirror test only demonstrates that some animals possess a particular cognitive capacity for modeling their environment, but not for the presence of phenomenal consciousness per se. Granting consciousness to animals might require demonstrations of thought-directed self-awareness, such as metacognition.

Some critics, such as philosopher Stuart Smith, maintain that it does not establish the existence of self-awareness of an independent character in animals whose self-awareness is solely a product of external experience.

Furthermore, even visually oriented creatures may not be familiar enough with mirrors to pass the test, or may not be motivated to touch a mark on their forehead for any number of reasons. Thus, Gallup's mirror test has been criticized as logically invalid because negative results are uninterpretable. Prosopagnosiacs, for example, may fail the test despite having the ability to report self awareness.

Finally, more and more people tend to minimize the importance of the mirror test since. Humans primary method of identification is trough visual observation. But that is not the case with all the animals. Cats for example, rely more on odor and sound than sight to identify objects or other animals. Also, since their flexibility allows them to view any part of their body, it's not hard to understand why they wouldn't really be all that interested in a mere reflection if it has no smell or sound. For the cat's mind, the incomplete sensory stimuli provided by the mirror, an object a cat would never encounter in the wild, may compare to that of a television screen which most house cats are already familiar with.

Criticisms

Despite the popularity of the mirror test, Asendorpf et al. (1996) have identified some drawbacks to solely relying on the mirror test as means of self-awareness for young children. In their 1996 study, they observed 109 children in Munich, Germany. They had an average age of 18.6 months. They tested 45 children using the classic mirror rouge test and an additional 59 children using modified mirror test. In the classic test, the experimenter first played with the children, making sure that they looked in the mirror at least three times. After the mirror recognition, the parent placed a large dot of rouge below the child’s right eye. The experimenter then brought the child back in front of the mirror until the child had three more instances of mirror recognition. Once in front of the mirror, the experimenter asked the child, “who is that?” up to three times, if needed, in order get a response. For the modified version of the mirror test, the experimenter introduced a doll with a rouge spot under its eye, and asked the children to help clean the doll. The experiment would ask the child for their help up to three times before cleaning the doll themselves. The doll was then put away and the experimenter played with the child for at least one minute. Then the parent applied the dot of rouge and the experimenter pointed out the mark on the child’s face. They were asked by the experimenter up to three times to clean the dot off of their face.

The results uncovered by Asendorpf et al. (1996) proved some issues with the classic mirror test. Their main problem with the classic mirror test is that it assumes that children will recognize the dot of rouge and in recognizing that it’s not normal, try to get rid of it. Asendorpf et al. (1996) found evidence against this. They found that the classic mirror test produced false negatives because the child’s recognition of the dot did not lead to them cleaning it. Therefore, the researchers have assumed that the children may not have developed self-recognition skills yet. In their modified test, in which the doll was cleaned first, they found a stronger relationship between cleaning the doll’s face and the child cleaning their own face. The demonstration with the doll means that showing the children what to do first may lead to more reliable instances of self-recognition. [21]

See also

References

  1. ^ Gallup, GG Jr. (1970). "Chimpanzees: Self recognition". Science 167 (3914): 86–87. doi:10.1126/science.167.3914.86. PMID 4982211. 
  2. ^ Psychologist May 1977
  3. ^ a b c d e f Stanley Coren. How dogs think. ISBN 0743222326. 
  4. ^ a b c d e Archer, John (1992). Ethology and Human Development. Rowman & Littlefield. ISBN 0389209961. 
  5. ^ a b Miller, Jason (2009). "Minding the Animals: Ethology and the Obsolescence of Left Humanism". American Chronicle. http://www.americanchronicle.com/articles/view/102661. Retrieved 2009-05-21. 
  6. ^ Povinelli, Daniel; de Veer, Monique; Gallup Jr., Gordon; Theall, Laura; van den Bos, Ruud (2003). "An 8-year longitudinal study of mirror self-recognition in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)". Neuropsychologia 41 (2): 229–334. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00153-7. ISSN 0028-3932. 
  7. ^ "National Geographic documentary "Human Ape"". http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=W-pc_M2qI74. Retrieved 2010-06-11. 
  8. ^ Marten, K. & Psarakos, S. (1995). "Evidence of self-awareness in the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus)". In Parker, S.T., Mitchell, R. & Boccia, M.. Self-awareness in Animals and Humans: Developmental Perspectives. Cambridge University Press. pp. 361–379. http://earthtrust.org/delbook.html. Retrieved 2008-10-04. 
  9. ^ Delfour, F; Marten, K (2001). "Mirror image processing in three marine mammal species: killer whales (Orcinus orca), false killer whales (Pseudorca crassidens) and California sea lions (Zalophus californianus)". Behavioural processes 53 (3): 181–190. PMID 11334706. 
  10. ^ Joshua M. Plotnik, Frans B.M. de Waal, and Diana Reiss (2006) Self-recognition in an Asian elephant. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 103(45):17053–17057 10.1073/pnas.0608062103 abstract
  11. ^ Prior, Helmut; Schwarz, Ariane; Güntürkün, Onur; De Waal, Frans (2008). De Waal, Frans. ed. "Mirror-Induced Behavior in the Magpie (Pica pica): Evidence of Self-Recognition". PLoS Biology (Public Library of Science) 6 (8): e202. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0060202. PMC 2517622. PMID 18715117. http://biology.plosjournals.org/archive/1545-7885/6/8/pdf/10.1371_journal.pbio.0060202-L.pdf. Retrieved 2008-08-21. 
  12. ^ Francine Patterson and Wendy Gordon The Case for Personhood of Gorillas. In The Great Ape Project, ed. Paola Cavalieri and Peter Singer, St. Martin's Griffin, 1993, pp. 58–77.
  13. ^ "Consciousness and the Symbolic Universe"
  14. ^ Keim, Brandon (2010). "Monkeys See Selves in Mirror, Open a Barrel of Questions". Wired. http://www.wired.com/wiredscience/2010/09/monkey-self-awareness/. Retrieved 2010-10-01. 
  15. ^ Epstein, Lanza; Skinner, RP; Skinner, BF (1981). ""Self-awareness" in the pigeon". Science 212 (4495): 695–696. doi:10.1126/science.212.4495.695. PMID 17739404. 
  16. ^ http://psychology.lafayette.edu/files/2010/06/selfaware.mov is video of one such test
  17. ^ Frans B.M. de Waal. "The Thief in the Mirror". PLoS Biology (Public Library of Science). doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0060201. http://biology.plosjournals.org/perlserv/?request=get-pdf&file=10.1371_journal.pbio.0060201-L.pdf. Retrieved 2009-01-06. 
  18. ^ Roma, Peter; Silberberg, Alan; Huntsberry, Mary; Christensen, Chesley; Ruggiero, Angela; Suomi, Stephen (25 January 2007). "Mark tests for mirror self-recognition in capuchin monkeys (Cebus Apella) trained to touch marks". American Journal of Primatology 69 (9): 989–1000. doi:10.1002/ajp.20404. ISSN 0275-2565. PMID 17253635. "The results are consistent with the finding that no monkey species is capable of spontaneous mirror self-recognition." 
  19. ^ Anderson, James; Kuroshima, Hika; Paukner, Annika; Fujita, Kazuo (24 June 2008). "Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) respond to video images of themselves". Animal Cognition 12 (1): 55–62. doi:10.1007/s10071-008-0170-3. ISSN 1435-9448. PMID 18574604. "Although they showed no signs of explicit self-recognition, the monkeys' behaviour strongly suggests recognition of the correspondence between kinaesthetic information and external visual effects." 
  20. ^ Broom, D. M.; Sena, H.; Moynihan, K. L. (2009). "Pigs learn what a mirror image represents and use it to obtain information". Animal Behaviour 78 (5): 1037. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.07.027.  edit
  21. ^ Asendorpf, J. B., Warkentin, V., & Baudonniere, P.-M. (1996). Self-Awareness and Other Awareness II: Mirror Self-Recognition, Social Contigency Awareness, and Synchronic Imitation. Developmental Psychology , 32 (2), 313-321.

External links